久久精品精选,精品九九视频,www久久只有这里有精品,亚洲熟女乱色综合一区
    分享

    第407篇Infant-Directed Speech

     托福真題網 2025-02-05 發布于山東

    Infant-Directed Speech

    Infant-directed speech is a style of speech directed toward infants. This type of speech pattern was previously called motherese, because it was assumed that it applied only to mothers. However, that assumption was wrong, and the gender-neutral term infant- directed speech is now used more frequently.

    Infant-directed speech is characterized by short, simple sentences. Pitch (the highness or lowness of a sound) becomes generally higher, the range of frequencies (essentially, the difference between the highest and the lowest pitch used) increases, and intonation (the rise and fall in pitch) is more varied. There is also repetition of words, and topics are restricted to items that are assumed to be comprehensible to infants, such as concrete objects in the baby's environment. Sometimes infant-directed speech includes amusing sounds that are not even words, imitating the prelinguistic speech of infants. In other cases, it has little formal structure but is similar to the kind of telegraphic speech that infants use as they develop their own language skills.

    Infant-directed speech changes as children become older. Around the end of the first year, infant-directed speech takes on more adultlike qualities. Sentences become longer and more complex, although individual words are still spoken slowly and deliberately. Pitch is also used to focus attention on particularly important words.

    Infant-directed speech plays an important role in infants' acquisition of language. Newborns respond to such speech more readily than they do to regular language, a fact that suggests that they may be particularly receptive to it. Furthermore, some research suggests that unusually extensive exposure to infant-directed speech early in life is related to the comparatively early appearance of first words and earlier linguistic competence in other areas.

    Is infant-directed speech similar across cultures? In some respects, it clearly is. According to a growing body of research, there are basic similarities across cultures in the nature of infant-directed speech. Consider, for instance, a comparison of the major characteristics of speech directed at infants used by native speakers of English and Spanish. Of the ten most frequent features, six are common to both: exaggerated intonation, high pitch, lengthened vowels, repetition, lower volume, and instructional emphasis (that is, heavy stress on certain key words, such as emphasizing the word "ball" in the sentence, "No, that's not a ball."). Similarly, mothers in the United States, Sweden, and Russia all exaggerate and elongate the pronunciation of the three vowel sounds of "ee," "ah," and "oh" when speaking to infants in similar ways, despite differences in the languages in which the sounds are used. Even deaf mothers use a form of infant-directed speech: When communicating with their infants, deaf mothers use sign language at a significantly slower tempo than when communicating with adults, and they frequently repeat the signs.

    The cross-cultural similarities in infant-directed speech are so great, in fact, that they appear in some facets of language specific to particular types of interactions. For instance, evidence comparing American English, German, and Mandarin Chinese speakers shows that in each of the languages, pitch rises when a mother is attempting to get an infant's attention or produce a response, while pitch falls when she is trying to calm an infant. Why do we find such similarities across very different languages? One hypothesis is that the characteristics of infant-directed speech activate innate responses in infants. As we have noted, infants seem to prefer infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech, suggesting that their perceptual systems may be more responsive to such characteristics. Another explanation is that infant-directed speech facilitates language development, providing cues as to the meaning of speech before infants have developed the capacity to understand the meanings of words.

    Despite the similarities in the style of infant-directed speech across diverse cultures, there are some important cultural differences in the quantity of speech that infants hear from their parents. For example, although the Gusii of Kenya care for their infants in an extremely close, physical way, they speak to them less than American parents do. 

    1

    ?Infant-directed speech is a style of speech directed toward infants. This type of speech pattern was previously called motherese, because it was assumed that it applied only to mothers. However, that assumption was wrong, and the gender-neutral term infant- directed speech is now used more frequently. 

    2

    ?Infant-directed speech is characterized by short, simple sentences. Pitch (the highness or lowness of a sound) becomes generally higher, the range of frequencies (essentially, the difference between the highest and the lowest pitch used) increases, and intonation (the rise and fall in pitch) is more varied. There is also repetition of words, and topics are restricted to items that are assumed to be comprehensible to infants, such as concrete objects in the baby's environment. Sometimes infant-directed speech includes amusing sounds that are not even words, imitating the prelinguistic speech of infants. In other cases, it has little formal structure but is similar to the kind of telegraphic speech that infants use as they develop their own language skills. 

    3

    ?Infant-directed speech changes as children become older. Around the end of the first year, infant-directed speech takes on more adultlike qualities. Sentences become longer and more complex, although individual words are still spoken slowly and deliberately. Pitch is also used to focus attention on particularly important words. 

    4

    ?Infant-directed speech plays an important role in infants' acquisition of language. Newborns respond to such speech more readily than they do to regular language, a fact that suggests that they may be particularly receptive to it. Furthermore, some research suggests that unusually extensive exposure to infant-directed speech early in life is related to the comparatively early appearance of first words and earlier linguistic competence in other areas.

    5

    ?Is infant-directed speech similar across cultures? In some respects, it clearly is. According to a growing body of research, there are basic similarities across cultures in the nature of infant-directed speech. Consider, for instance, a comparison of the major characteristics of speech directed at infants used by native speakers of English and Spanish. Of the ten most frequent features, six are common to both: exaggerated intonation, high pitch, lengthened vowels, repetition, lower volume, and instructional emphasis (that is, heavy stress on certain key words, such as emphasizing the word "ball" in the sentence, "No, that's not a ball."). Similarly, mothers in the United States, Sweden, and Russia all exaggerate and elongate the pronunciation of the three vowel sounds of "ee," "ah," and "oh" when speaking to infants in similar ways, despite differences in the languages in which the sounds are used. Even deaf mothers use a form of infant-directed speech: When communicating with their infants, deaf mothers use sign language at a significantly slower tempo than when communicating with adults, and they frequently repeat the signs. 

    6

    ?Is infant-directed speech similar across cultures? In some respects, it clearly is. According to a growing body of research, there are basic similarities across cultures in the nature of infant-directed speech. Consider, for instance, a comparison of the major characteristics of speech directed at infants used by native speakers of English and Spanish. Of the ten most frequent features, six are common to both: exaggerated intonation, high pitch, lengthened vowels, repetition, lower volume, and instructional emphasis (that is, heavy stress on certain key words, such as emphasizing the word "ball" in the sentence, "No, that's not a ball."). Similarly, mothers in the United States, Sweden, and Russia all exaggerate and elongate the pronunciation of the three vowel sounds of "ee," "ah," and "oh" when speaking to infants in similar ways, despite differences in the languages in which the sounds are used. Even deaf mothers use a form of infant-directed speech: When communicating with their infants, deaf mothers use sign language at a significantly slower tempo than when communicating with adults, and they frequently repeat the signs. 

    7

    ?The cross-cultural similarities in infant-directed speech are so great, in fact, that they appear in some facets of language specific to particular types of interactions. For instance, evidence comparing American English, German, and Mandarin Chinese speakers shows that in each of the languages, pitch rises when a mother is attempting to get an infant's attention or produce a response, while pitch falls when she is trying to calm an infant. Why do we find such similarities across very different languages? One hypothesis is that the characteristics of infant-directed speech activate innate responses in infants. As we have noted, infants seem to prefer infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech, suggesting that their perceptual systems may be more responsive to such characteristics. Another explanation is that infant-directed speech facilitates language development, providing cues as to the meaning of speech before infants have developed the capacity to understand the meanings of words. 

    8

    ?The cross-cultural similarities in infant-directed speech are so great, in fact, that they appear in some facets of language specific to particular types of interactions. For instance, evidence comparing American English, German, and Mandarin Chinese speakers shows that in each of the languages, pitch rises when a mother is attempting to get an infant's attention or produce a response, while pitch falls when she is trying to calm an infant. Why do we find such similarities across very different languages? One hypothesis is that the characteristics of infant-directed speech activate innate responses in infants. As we have noted, infants seem to prefer infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech, suggesting that their perceptual systems may be more responsive to such characteristics. Another explanation is that infant-directed speech facilitates language development, providing cues as to the meaning of speech before infants have developed the capacity to understand the meanings of words. 

    9

    Is infant-directed speech similar across cultures? In some respects, it clearly is. According to a growing body of research, there are basic similarities across cultures in the nature of infant-directed speech. ? Consider, for instance, a comparison of the major characteristics of speech directed at infants used by native speakers of English and Spanish. ? Of the ten most frequent features, six are common to both: exaggerated intonation, high pitch, lengthened vowels, repetition, lower volume, and instructional emphasis (that is, heavy stress on certain key words, such as emphasizing the word "ball" in the sentence, "No, that's not a ball."). ? Similarly, mothers in the United States, Sweden, and Russia all exaggerate and elongate the pronunciation of the three vowel sounds of "ee," "ah," and "oh" when speaking to infants in similar ways, despite differences in the languages in which the sounds are used. ? Even deaf mothers use a form of infant-directed speech: When communicating with their infants, deaf mothers use sign language at a significantly slower tempo than when communicating with adults, and they frequently repeat the signs. 

    10

      本站是提供個人知識管理的網絡存儲空間,所有內容均由用戶發布,不代表本站觀點。請注意甄別內容中的聯系方式、誘導購買等信息,謹防詐騙。如發現有害或侵權內容,請點擊一鍵舉報。
      轉藏 分享 獻花(0

      0條評論

      發表

      請遵守用戶 評論公約

      類似文章 更多

      主站蜘蛛池模板: 精品无码人妻一区二区三区| 亚洲成AV人片在线观看麦芽| 国产明星精品无码AV换脸| 国产无人区码一区二区| 亚洲国产精品成人AV在线| 国产一区二区波多野结衣| 亚洲AV无一区二区三区| 免费又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片 | 337P日本欧洲亚洲大胆精品| 日韩精品一区二区三区视频| 国产第一页浮力影院入口| 四虎永久免费高清视频| 国产精品丝袜亚洲熟女| 久久精品这里热有精品| 亚洲A成人片在线播放| 天堂mv在线mv免费mv香蕉| 美女被强奷到抽搐的动态图| 国产又黄又湿又刺激网站| 精品乱码无人区一区二区 | 日韩欧美亚洲综合久久| 国产欧美久久久精品影院| 老司机午夜精品视频资源| 无码人妻丰满熟妇啪啪网不卡| 国产精品青青在线观看爽香蕉| 亚洲日韩欧洲无码AV夜夜摸| 香港日本三级亚洲三级| 亚洲国产成人无码AV在线影院L| 好吊视频一区二区三区| 国产精品自在拍首页视频8| 人妻少妇久久中文字幕| 亚洲最大成人免费av| 狠狠色丁香婷婷综合潮喷| 白丝乳交内射一二三区| 国产一区二区在线影院| 美女自卫慰黄网站| 强奷漂亮少妇高潮麻豆| 另类专区一区二区三区| 精品久久久久久无码人妻热| 2020国产激情视频在线观看| 欧美国产日韩A在线观看 | 99九九视频高清在线|